USC Advanced Undergraduate Phonology ✳︎ Fall 2019 ✳︎ Smith
The challenge
- In rule-and-constraint based phonology, to describe a language you need:
- an ordered list of rules
- a list of URs (the lexicon)
- a phoneme inventory
- maybe restrictions on combinations of segments in URs (called Morpheme Structure Constraint (MSCs))
- maybe restrictions on the segments allowed in outputs or derivations
- called “Wellformedness constraints” or “Surface phonetic constraints”
- Some of the restrictions above seem redundant: the Duplication Problem.
- In OT, all you have is…
- a list of URs (the lexicon)
- a ranked list of constraints
- The lack of an underlying alphabet – or any constraints on the structure of URs – is a property called Richness of the Base.
- So how can we account for which sounds do and don’t contrast, without stating the phoneme inventory?
- How do we state the inventory, generally?
- This seems potentially problematic given Richness of the Base…
- Probably not surprising: inventories and contrast come solely from constraint ranking.
- Contrast in OT comes from faithfulness constraints, which preserve contrast in a certain environment.
- Markedness constraints militate against the faithful realization of inputs containing sounds illicit in a language.
Possible patterns of contrast
- No contrast
- How do we know it when we see it?
- Full contrast
- How do we know it when we see it?
- Contextually limited contrast
- How do we know it when we see it?
- How does this relate to positional neutralization?
- No contrast - free variation
- How do we know it when we see it?
- The version of OT we’re considering isn’t built to model it.
- EVAL results in a single optimal candidate, or multiple optimal candidates if they have identical violation profiles.
Complementary distribution between sounds (no contrast)
- Imagine a language where [p] and [b] are in complementary distribution, and allophones of a single phoneme.
- In rule terms:
- /p/ → [b] / V__V no phoneme /b/
- /p/ → [p] elsewhere (since there’s no rule)
- Why is [b] only between vowels?
- Why is [p] everywhere else?
- Translate into OT using:
- *V[-voice]V
- *[+voice,-son]
- Ident([voice])
- Assume these inputs
- /ap/ ‘dog’ (real word)
- /ap+a/ ‘dogs’ (real word)
- /ab/ ‘?’ (hypothetical input)
- /ab+a/ ‘?(plural)’ (hypothetical input)
- /p/ and /b/ don’t contrast, because the grammar won’t let you have a minimal pair.
- What is it about the ranking that makes that so?
Full contrast between sounds
- Imagine a different language, where /p/ and /b/ are separate phonemes, with no voicing alternations.
- Same constraints
- *V[-voice]V
- *[+voice,-son]
- Ident([voice])
- Find the ranking for:
- /ap/ ‘dog’
- /ap+a/ ‘dogs’
- /ab/ ‘cat’
- /ab+a/ ‘cats’
- /p/ and /b/ contrast fully-you can get a minimal pair in any environment.
- What is it about the ranking that makes that so?
Contextually-limited contrast (positional neutralization)
- Imagine language with a voicing contrast (phonemes /p/ and /b/), but also intervocalic voicing.
- In rule terms:
- /p/ → [b] / V__V
- /p/ → [p] elsewhere
- /b/ → [b] everywhere
- Same constraints
- *V[-voice]V
- *[+voice,-son]
- Ident([voice])
- Find the ranking for:
- /ap/ ‘dog’
- /ap+a/ ‘dogs’
- /ab/ ‘cat’
- /ab+a/ ‘cats’
- /p/ & /b/ contrast in some environments – but in other environments, you get neutralization.
- What is it about the ranking that makes that so?
- Have we missed any members of the factorial typology for these three constraints?
- There are 3! rankings – any other patterns?
Contextually limited contrast two ways
- Example: coda devoicing (German, Catalan, Russian, etc.)
- In onsets, obstruents contrast in voicing.
- That contrast does not occur in coda position.
- Results in both positional neutralization and contextually-limited contrast.
- Two approaches in OT:
- Context-specific markedness constraints
- Cause neutralization in a particular environment
- Example: *Coda-voice
- Contextually-limited faithfulness constraints
- Preserve contrast in a particular environment
- Example: Ident-onset([voice])
Contextually-limited contrast as contextual faithfulness
- Beckman (1998) takes another approach to contextually limited contrast
- She proposes positional faithfulness constraints, now standard in OT analyses.
- Contrast is preserved in a context X because a faithfulness constraint targets segments in that context.
- Common contexts where contrast is preserved: stressed syllables, initial syllables, onsets: all contexts that are perceptually salient.
- Preservation of voicing in onsets could be captured by the constraint below:
- Ident-onset-([voice]) Let Y be an output segment in the onset, and X the corresponding input segment. Assign one violation if Y is [αvoice] and X is not [αvoice].
- Let’s work this out for some hypothetical inputs:
- /bad/ → [bat]
- /bad-e/ → [bade]
- /gat/ → [gat]
- /gat-e/ → [gate]
- We can also pursue an alternative analysis using a context-specific markedness constraint like *Coda-voice.
- Could we use positional faithfulness for the case of neutralization in intervocalic voicing? Let’s sketch an analysis.
Lexicon optimization
- Does Richness of the Base mean anything can be in underlying representations?
- In theory, yes – there are no hard limits on URs.
- Does Richness of the Base mean underlying representations in a language include everything?
- For an actual language, no – learners won’t posit “weird” or “crazy” URs without evidence.
- Prince & Smolensky (1993/2004) suggest a learning procedure called Lexicon Optimization.
- For a given phonetic form, the chosen UR will be the one that maps onto the SR with the fewest violation of highest-ranked constraints.
- In the absence of aleternations, learners will pick a UR that gives a mapping minimizing the violation of faithfulness constraints: the UR matches the SR.
- This runs contrary to the idea that we should minimize the number of underlying sounds in a language, and it especially runs contrary to the usage of abstract phonemes to minimize the number of underlying sounds.
- An example: imagine a non-alternating phonological property – like the aspiration of initial [kʰ] in [kʰæt] in English.
- The correct grammar for English will ensure the surface occurrence of [kʰ] in [kʰæt] no matter whether [kʰ] or [k] is present underlyingly.
- The advantage of the mapping /kʰæt/ ➞ [kʰæt] over /kæt/ ➞ [kæt] is that the former avoids a faithfulness violation, e.g. Ident([spread glottis]).
- So learners will posit the mapping /kʰæt/ ➞ [kʰæt].
- Non-alternating phonological properties will always be present in URs.
Recap of important ideas
- Capturing contrast in OT
- Richness of the Base
- Lexicon Optimization
- Contextually-limited contrast as…
- Contextual markedness
- Positional faithfulness